Why Are Gods Thought to Cause Weather?

Gods and Climate

Carol R. Ember and Ian Skoggard

In many societies known to anthropology, people look to their gods for help and for explanations of unfortunate and traumatic events. Anthropologist Bronislaw Malinowski (1948) suggested that religion is universal because life is filled with unavoidable stresses and humans need the comfort and relief provided by religion. But while his theory may plausibly explain the universality of religious belief, it does not explain the structure or form that religion takes nor does it explain more specific religious beliefs, such as why people might believe that their gods can influence weather to help or hurt them.

For the last six years, the HRAF team has pursued research on how living in hazard-prone environments may have influenced and transformed culture. Might these environments have affected religious belief? More specifically, we asked whether religions might differ in societies that have more food-destroying hazards, such as drought or floods. Also, we asked more broadly whether the important factors might be the experience of resource stress or living in difficult climates.

If religion does provide comfort and relief to people, it follows that societies with more resource uncertainty might believe that their gods could help them, or, at least provide an explanation of why they are experiencing misfortune. Help could take the form of bringing needed rain, or quieting damaging winds. But some societies also believe that gods can hurt people with bad weather. Gods and spirits might be mischievous or capricious, or sometimes deliberately punish people for behavior they consider bad.

An example of benign godly involvement with weather is found among the Pawnee who believed in an omnipresent deity, Tirawahat, who sent storms to cool the fields and cause corn and timber to grow (Chamberlain 1982). The Burmese believed in spirits called nats that often acted capriciously effecting weather events, such as dry spells (Spiro 1978). Another more personal example is from Skoggard’s trip to Ethiopia during the rainy season except that it was not raining in one village he visited. A concerned priest had plans to hold a palaver to discern if there were any altercations among villagers such as unsocial behavior that might have displeased god and caused god to hold back the rain.

Methodology & Results

To test our ideas, we first coded four types of godly belief for a sample of 98 societies, largely by reading ethnographic passages from eHRAF World Cultures. We looked at different types of gods and spirits (high gods or supreme beings, superior gods, and minor spirits) and, for each type, asked the following questions: Is the type of god or spirit believed to be associated with weather in some way? Is the type of god or spirit believed to be able to help a people’s food supply with weather? Is the type of god or spirit believed to hurt food supply with weather? And is the type of god or spirit believed to use weather to punish people for unacceptable behavior?  We also worked with a climatologist team to develop climate measures. These measures helped us assess the likelihood of drought, flood, extreme heat or extreme cold.

Our results were the clearest for beliefs about high gods (a supreme being). Our data support the idea that societies with more resource stress, such as those with food-destroying hazards, famine, and chronic scarcity, are more likely to believe that their high god is involved with weather in some way. Somewhat surprisingly, high gods are rarely seen as only helping or only hurting people with weather. They usually are believed to do both, although – to be sure – at different times.

Our climate results suggest that not all extremes of climate are associated with the belief that high gods are associated with weather. For example, extremes of temperature – unusually hot or unusually cold – do not appear to predict such high god beliefs. And extremes in precipitation only predict in one direction: extreme dryness is predictive of such beliefs, but extreme wetness predicts less of such beliefs.

Church window detailWhy should only extremes of dryness predict the belief that high gods are involved with weather? We suggested that extreme dryness is probably more anxiety-arousing than extreme wetness (Ember et al. 2021). The Nahua of Mexico say, “Water is our life. It is what gives us life. Without water, we would die’’ (Taggart 2019, p. 49), suggesting that too little water is more frightening than too much water. Societies have adapted to drought through practices such as planting drought-resistant crops, storing grain in case of crop failure, and moving when necessary, but drought is a slow-onset event that can affect wide regions and can persist for years beyond the capacity of mitigation efforts. But apart from its effects on food supply, lack of water is immediately threatening to health and life (Rosinger and Brewis 2020; Schuster et al. 2020).

In contrast, extreme wet events such as flooding, are usually fast-onset events that mostly affect people living near rivers or coastal areas. And many societies have adapted to flooding through technological infrastructure such as houses on stilts, raised fields, extra drainage channels, and locating settlements on high ground. Obviously, societies with too much rain will not lack for drinking water. Similarly, like flood events, extreme cold and extreme heat events are usually not long-lasting and people are able to mitigate their effects more easily.

Does resource stress and extreme dryness work together to produce beliefs that high gods are associated with weather? Since drought is likely to cause resource stress, we tested a causal model suggesting that climate may only have an indirect effect on high god beliefs. That is, climate affects resource stress, which in turn, affects high god beliefs. Surprisingly, our results do not support the indirect effect of climate change. First, while our results are consistent with the idea that climate influences resource stress, climate also appears to have separate and independent effects on beliefs that high gods are associated with weather. Two possible explanations are that extreme weather events can still cause loss of life even if they do not destroy food supply. Also, such events, while not disastrous, might evoke memories of past traumatic climate-caused disasters and induce fears and subsequent appeals for supernatural aid and comfort.

Another aspect of religion we focused on is the belief in moralizing high gods. A moralizing high god is a god that people believe judges their behavior and metes out punishments or rewards depending upon whether they behave appropriately. There is much scholarly interest in the possible effect of moralizing high god beliefs on the ability of societies to foster coordination and cooperation, particularly in more complex societies.

Because we had previously published research suggesting that food and labor sharing between households was more likely in societies experiencing more resource stress (Ember et al. 2018), we decided to test the idea that the presence of moralizing high gods might help explain such sharing, an important form of cooperation. Perhaps people with resource stress are more likely to believe in moralizing high gods and that religious belief in turn favors increased sharing. But our results did not support the role of moralizing high gods in predicting sharing. Rather, our findings are consistent with the idea that resource stress is more important. It not only appears to directly increase certain kinds of religious belief, such as the idea that high gods are associated with weather and are moralizing, but also resource stress, independent of religious belief, predicts more beyond-household sharing. In short, resource stress itself appears to be the more important variable predicting both religious belief and sharing.

As always, research raises more questions to try to answer in the future. Is there evidence that drought is more anxiety arousing that flooding, or extreme heat or cold? Are people who believe that their high gods affect weather less anxious in the face of severe weather-related events? Does climate influence religion in other ways? Does religion provide important coping skills in face of climate change and extreme weather events?

To learn more about the research described here, see Ember et al. 2018, Skoggard et al. 2020, and Ember et al. 2021.

 

References

Ember, C. R., Skoggard, I., Felzer, B., Pitek, E., & Jiang, M. 2021. Climate variability, drought, and the belief that high gods are associated with weather in nonindustrial societies. Weather, Climate, and Society, 13(2), 259-272. https://doi.org/10.1175/WCAS-D-20-0080.1.

Ember, C.R., Skoggard, I., Ringen, E.J. and Farrer, M., 2018. Our better nature: Does resource stress predict beyond-household sharing?. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(4), pp.380-391. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.03.001

Chamberlain, V.D. 1982. When stars came down to earth: Cosmology of the Skidi Pawnee Indians of North America. Ballena Press Anthropological Papers. Los Altos: Ballena Press

Malinowski, B., 1948. Magic, Science and Religion. Doubleday.

Rosinger, A. Y., and A. Brewis, 2020. Life and death: Toward a human biology of water. American Journal of Human Biology, 32, e23361,https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.23361.

Schuster, R. C., M. S. Butler, A.Wutich, J. D. Miller, and S. L. Young. 2020. If there is no water, we cannot feed our children: The farreaching consequences of water insecurity on infant feeding practices and infant health across 16 low- and middle-income countries. American Journal of Human Biology, 32, e23357. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.23357.

Skoggard, I., Ember, C.R., Pitek, E. Jackson, J.C. and Carolus, C.  2020. Resource stress predicts changes in religious belief and increases in sharing behavior. Human Nature, 31, 249–271 https://doi.org/10.1175/WCAS-D-20-0080.1

Spiro, M.E. 1978. Burmese Supernaturalism. Philadelphia: Institute for the Study of Human Relations.

Taggart, J. 2019. The Rain Gods’ Rebellion: The Cultural Basis of a Nahua Insurgency. University Press of Colorado.